Spaced Practice – an Essential for Effective Learning

Table of Contents

What is Spaced Practice?

Spaced practice refers to spreading your study or practice sessions across days. The alternative is massed practice, which refers to practicing the same amount of time but all at once (e.g., on a single day).1

Spaced Practice vs. Massed Practice

Spaced practice can easily be applied to both procedural and declarative learning. 2,3

Spaced practice may seem like the obviously correct thing to do — after all, most skills worth learning take longer than a day to master, so it may seem like there’s no choice.

But in the real world, people tend to manage their time poorly and “cram” (which is essentially what massed practice is). This is unfortunate, because as you’ll see below, spaced practice is routinely proven to be more effective for both short and long term learning.

Interleaved (or Random) Practice. Spacing is often involved in interleaving, but doesn’t refer to the same thing as spaced practice.1 What’s more, you could apply blocked practice within a spaced practice session (practicing the same thing again and again, but all within a given practice session — and you could still do that spaced throughout the week). We will learn more about Interleaved Practice in a future update.

Does it Work? What the Science Says

The following summarizes the results of research studies on spaced practice (compared to massed practice). For help interpreting tables, see definitions below.

Procedural Learning

Procedural learning refers to learning “how to” information, rather than learning facts.4 Often studied using movement tasks (motor learning).

Spaced Practice Compared to Massed Practice

Performance Learning
Effect Size Reliability Effect Size Reliability
Overall
medium
Medium
High
High
medium
Medium
High
High
Simple Movements
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Complex Movements
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Sequenced Movements
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member
Locked Become a Member

Performance: refers to the effect of the practice or study technique on improving immediate performance (also referred to as “acquisition”) — i.e., right after you finish practicing. This is important, because some practice or study techniques will cause you to perform worse at first, but over the long term, you will learn more.

Learning: refers to the effect of this technique on longer term learning (also referred to as “retention”) — i.e., a week, month, or year later. Studies vary in the “follow up time” they test to differentiate immediate performance and long-term learning, but it usually doesn’t matter: if learning is superior in two weeks, it’s usually also superior months later.

For an excellent review of the performance-learning distinction, refer to Soderstrom & Bjork (2015).5

Notes on Spaced vs. Massed Practice:

  • Studies are usually designed so that there’s an acquisition “phase” (e.g., a week) where the spaced group would do a few sessions on different days, and the massed group would do everything on one day.
  • The “acquisition” test would be done immediately after training. This would test “Performance”.
    • Note that because of this design, the spaced practice group has already benefitted from some “retention” effects, but the massed practice group has not.
    • This may seem “unfair” but remember that the point of these studies is to look at the benefits of structuring your practice schedule one way or the other — controlling for total practice time, what’s better?
  • Then a week or two, or a month, or even a year later a “retention” test would be performed, which tests “Learning”.

Effect Size: refers to “how much” something works.6 That is, effect sizes refer to the magnitude of the difference between this technique and something else (usually doing the opposite; e.g., most studies on spaced practice will compare it to massed practice).

Reliability: refers to how reliable this technique is for most people, most of the time. Everyone will respond slightly differently, and some techniques are inherently more variable in their efficacy. This is important for determining whether something is worth trying, and managing expectations. For the science and stats nerds out there: this includes “statistical significance” and the quality of the study design — I’ll write about how I synthesize these things another day.

Overall: This row in the table combines all the data, whether it’s a simple or complex movement or sequence of movements, beginner or expert effects, and so on. If you don’t see a more specific row below, this helps you make some general assumptions: “is it effective for my very specific skill?” — if “Overall” says yes, then the answer is “probably”. But if you have a sense of what type of skill you’re going after, it’s better to look at the more specific rows below.

Simple vs. Complex Movements: I’ll make a separate page discussing how a movement can be defined as simple or complex. It’s debatable. But for now, I’m going to use the loose definition proposed by Wulf & Shea (2002)7:

  • Simple movements have few degrees of freedom — meaning there are fewer ways the body parts involved can move. They can often be learned in a single practice session (though perhaps not mastered), and are somewhat “artificial”, like experimental tasks with little real-world utility.
  • Complex movements have many degrees of freedom, take more time to learn (and far more time to master), and are often more “ecologically valid” — meaning they are relevant and useful in real-world settings.

Many (perhaps most) research studies in motor learning utilize simple tasks because they’re easy to control and measure, complex tasks often produce different results. Therefore, this is the probably most important distinction when aiming to apply this science to the real world.

Sequenced Movements: Movement sequences involve several “discrete” movements that must be completed in a particular order and/or with specific timing. Of course, the discrete movements involved can be simple (like typing on a keyboard) or complex (like dance choreography) — but few studies go into that level of complexity, and that’s not the most important point. The important point is: sequences are a different type of challenge to the performer.8 To oversimplify, it requires greater “cognitive” problem solving (like memorizing a phone number) than “motor” problem solving. Therefore, practice techniques will have different effects on sequence tasks. It’s an interesting topic that we will write more about soon. 

Spaced practice works well for procedural learning. With nearly 10,000 subjects across meta-analyses and replicated by more recent research, the result is highly reliable. Click the little “+” icon on each row of the table for sources.

The effect is solid: overall, a medium effect size for improving performance immediately after training, and slightly better when looking at longer term learning (retention) later. It seems clear from the research that spaced practice is a good idea.

This alone should motivate you to plan your learning in advance: start early, and schedule multiple sessions ahead of a performance, game, test, or whatever the goal is. If you’re looking to master a skill, you likely already expect it to take a while — but remember that spreading practice out across a week is likely better than massing it on a single day each week (this is how most of the studies cited were designed).

Maecenas fermentum consequat mi. Excepteur sint occaecat cupidatat non proident, sunt in culpa qui officia deserunt mollit anim id est laborum. Nulla gravida orci a odio. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque. Nulla vitae mauris non felis mollis faucibus. Nullam varius, turpis et commodo pharetra, est eros bibendum elit, nec luctus magna felis sollicitudin mauris.

Curabitur pretium tincidunt lacus. Etiam tempor. Pellentesque malesuada nulla a mi. Excepteur sint occaecat cupidatat non proident, sunt in culpa qui officia deserunt mollit anim id est laborum.

Etiam tempor. Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat. Nulla gravida orci a odio. Cras mollis scelerisque nunc. Curabitur pretium tincidunt lacus.

Nulla vitae mauris non felis mollis faucibus. Lorem ipsum dolor sit amet, consectetur adipiscing elit. Pellentesque malesuada nulla a mi. Nulla vitae mauris non felis mollis faucibus.

Ut ullamcorper, ligula eu tempor congue, eros est euismod turpis, id tincidunt sapien risus a quam. Donec fermentum. Maecenas fermentum consequat mi. Donec fermentum. Pellentesque malesuada nulla a mi.

Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat. Duis aute irure dolor in reprehenderit in voluptate velit esse cillum dolore eu fugiat nulla pariatur. Donec fermentum. Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat.

Coming Soon: Declarative Learning

Declarative learning refers to the learning of facts, rather than “how-to”.4

This content is part of the roadmap and will be coming soon. Want to influence what comes next? Contact us and make a suggestion!

Coming Soon: Why Might Spaced Practice Work?

The neuroscience of spaced practice.

This content is part of the roadmap and will be coming soon. Want to influence what comes next? Contact us and make a suggestion!

How to Use Spaced Practice

Spaced practice can easily be applied simply by spreading practice or study sessions across the week. But there are a few things to keep in mind to maximize its effectiveness.

Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque. Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat. Etiam tempor. Nullam varius, turpis et commodo pharetra, est eros bibendum elit, nec luctus magna felis sollicitudin mauris. Duis aute irure dolor in reprehenderit in voluptate velit esse cillum dolore eu fugiat nulla pariatur. Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat.

Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat. Maecenas fermentum consequat mi. Maecenas fermentum consequat mi. Donec fermentum.

Sed do eiusmod tempor incididunt ut labore et dolore magna aliqua. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque. Donec fermentum. Nulla vitae mauris non felis mollis faucibus. Etiam tempor. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque. Duis ac tellus et risus vulputate vehicula.

Duis aute irure dolor in reprehenderit in voluptate velit esse cillum dolore eu fugiat nulla pariatur. Duis ac tellus et risus vulputate vehicula. Maecenas fermentum consequat mi. Etiam tempor.

Maecenas fermentum consequat mi. Ut ullamcorper, ligula eu tempor congue, eros est euismod turpis, id tincidunt sapien risus a quam. Aliquam faucibus, elit ut dictum aliquet, felis nisl adipiscing sapien, sed malesuada diam lacus eget erat. Donec lobortis risus a elit. Maecenas fermentum consequat mi.

Curabitur pretium tincidunt lacus. Curabitur pretium tincidunt lacus. Cras mollis scelerisque nunc. Etiam tempor. Lorem ipsum dolor sit amet, consectetur adipiscing elit.

Ut enim ad minim veniam, quis nostrud exercitation ullamco laboris nisi ut aliquip ex ea commodo consequat. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque. Ut ullamcorper, ligula eu tempor congue, eros est euismod turpis, id tincidunt sapien risus a quam. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque.

Donec lobortis risus a elit. Cras mollis scelerisque nunc. Duis aute irure dolor in reprehenderit in voluptate velit esse cillum dolore eu fugiat nulla pariatur. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque.

Duis ac tellus et risus vulputate vehicula. Donec lobortis risus a elit. Donec fermentum. Lorem ipsum dolor sit amet, consectetur adipiscing elit. Integer in mauris eu nibh euismod gravida.

Nulla vitae mauris non felis mollis faucibus. Cras mollis scelerisque nunc. Duis sapien sem, aliquet nec, commodo eget, consequat quis, neque. Cras mollis scelerisque nunc.

About the Author

Picture of Tony Ingram, PhD

Tony Ingram, PhD

Tony Ingram is a neuroscience PhD, ex-physical therapist, and former competitive dancer who writes about the science of learning skills.

References

Not including references included in the summary tables above. Click the “+” icon on each row of a table to see more references.

  1. Weinstein, Y., Madan, C. R., & Sumeracki, M. A. (2018). Teaching the science of learning. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3(1), 2. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-017-0087-y
  2. Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed Practice in Verbal Recall Tasks: A Review and Quantitative Synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 354–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354
  3. Lee, T. D., & Genovese, E. D. (1988). Distribution of Practice in Motor Skill Acquisition: Learning and Performance Effects Reconsidered. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 59(4), 277–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1988.10609373
  4. Squire, L. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005
  5. Soderstrom, N. C., & Bjork, R. A. (2015). Learning Versus Performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 176–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615569000
  6. Cumming, G. (2014). The New Statistics. Psychological Science, 25(1), 7–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613504966
  7. Wulf, G., & Shea, C. H. (2002). Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 185–211. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196276
  8. Krakauer, J. W., Hadjiosif, A. M., Xu, J., Wong, A. L., & Haith, A. M. (2019). Motor Learning. Comprehensive Physiology, 9(2), 613–663. https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c170043
  9. Wong, A. L., & Krakauer, J. W. (2019). Why Are Sequence Representations in Primary Motor Cortex So Elusive? Neuron, 103(6), 956–958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.011
  10. Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A Meta-Analytic Review of the Distribution of Practice Effect: Now You See It, Now You Don’t. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 795–805. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.795
  11. Sievertsen, H. H., Gino, F., & Piovesan, M. (2016). Cognitive fatigue influences students’ performance on standardized tests. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(10), 2621–2624. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1516947113
  12. Branscheidt, M., Kassavetis, P., Anaya, M., Rogers, D., Huang, H. D., Lindquist, M. A., & Celnik, P. (2019). Fatigue induces long-lasting detrimental changes in motor-skill learning. eLife, 8, e40578. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.40578